The 'travelling salesman problem' has puzzled mathematicians for
over eight decades, but bees might just have the answer. In a study published in September in the journal PLoS Biology, scientists show that bumblebees quickly
work out the shortest route to feed from several flowers and return to their
nest.
Worker bee with transponder (credit: Stephan Wolf from Rothamsted Research) |
Pollinator insects like bumblebees, and other foraging animals
such as hummingbirds, bats and even primates, establish stable routes between
regular feeding sites and their homes. To save energy, foragers have to come up
with a way of visiting multiple locations while traveling the shortest possible
distance. This is a task identical to the travelling salesman mathematical
problem, which tries to calculate the shortest route to visit several cities
once and return to the starting point. Now Lars Chittka's team at Queen Mary
University of London found how bees manage to solve this problem without
computers or even a map. Andy Reynolds from Rothamsted Research and a co-author in the
study says 'We showed how this complex routing problem can be solved by
small-brained animals without requiring 'map-like' memory'.
The scientists trained bees to collect sugar from artificial
flowers, and then followed their flying routes from the nest to five artificial
flowers arranged as a pentagon in a field. To do this, they attached a tiny
wire antenna to the back of each bee and tracked their movements with a radar,
much like a GPS navigation system. They found that the bees first visited the
nearest flowers to the nest, and that, in only eight round-trips, they had
discovered all the flowers. After this initial 'random' explorative phase, the
bees gradually began visiting the flowers in a specific sequence, as though in
each trip they were learning and progressively optimizing the 5-flower circuit.
Amazingly, in just about a couple dozen trips, each bee chose
the shortest possible route to visit all five flowers and return to the nest-
amongst the 120 other possible ways- and stuck to it 'Stable routes (...) that linked together all the flowers in an
optimal sequence were typically established after a bee made 26 foraging bouts,
during which time only about 20 of the 120 possible routes were tried.'
explains Reynolds.
Male bee with transponder (credit: Stephan Wolf from Rothamsted Research) |
But what happens if the flower spatial arrangement changes? In
the wild, bees have to modify their foraging routes in response to changes in
the environment. To understand how bees do this, the scientists removed an
artificial flower from the pentagonal experimental set-up and tracked the bees with
the radar. They found that the bees continued to visit all four flowers and the
empty feeding location using the optimal route, as if they could remember where
the missing flower had been.
Previous work on honeybees suggested that bees have a 'map-like
memory', but the authors in this study believe bees can develop optimal routes
simply by using 'a highly effective and versatile trial and error method'
Reynolds says. The scientists used their experimental data to develop a
mathematical 'trial and error' model based on heuristic, or experience-based,
algorithms. Similar heuristic models describing how ants find the shortest
routes between feeding locations and their nest are widely used by
mathematicians and computer scientists. These models work only for a low number
of locations, however, and in nature bees can feed from hundreds or even
thousands of flowers. So what happens then? Reynolds explains 'The trial-and-error model becomes impractical for 20 or more
locations but is effective for up to about 10 locations, which in practice
could facilitate the linking up of flower patches'.
Bees may move randomly between flowers within a flower patch,
but have a fixed order of flying between patches. 'This could be quite
effective because there could be much to gain by minimizing the distances flown
between patches but little to gain by minimizing the distances flown within
patches' he says.
The team had recently made similar observations in the
laboratory, but this is the first study examining the bee's routing behavior
over long distances and in a natural setting. Thomas Collett, a neurobiologist
at the University of Sussex who specializes in insect navigation says 'Such a study of the ontogeny of routes over the kinds of
distances that bumblebees normally fly has had to wait for the right
technology'.
In the future, Chittka's team would like to use their radar
tracking method to answer questions such as whether bees can solve the
travelling salesmen problem when more feeding sites are available. Collett says
'Testing [this] and other models will be exciting and may give new insights
into navigation and sequence learning'.
Source:
Lihoreau et al PLoS Biology (2012) DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001392
No comments:
Post a Comment